Bean CH (1938) The blind have “optical“ illusions. J Exp Psychol 22: 283-289 Blumenfeld W (1937) The relationship between the optical and haptic construction of space. Acta Psychologica 2: 133-157 Bruno N, Jacomuzzi A, Bertamini M, Meyer G (2007) A visual-haptic Necker cube reveals temporal constraints on intersensory merging during perceptual exploration. Neuropsychologia 45: 469-475 Caclin A, Soto-Faraco S, Kingstone A, Spence C (2002) Tactile capture of audition. Perception & Psychophysics 64: 616-630 Casla M, Blanco F, Traviesco D (1999) Haptic perception of geometric illusions by persons who are totally congenitally blind. J Visual Impairment 93: 583-588 Cheng MF (1968) Tactile-kinesthetic perception of length. Am J Psychol 81: 74-82 Congedo M, Lecuyer A, Gentaz E (2006) The influence of spatial de-location on perceptual integration of vision and touch. Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments 15: 353-357 Coren S, Girgus JS (1978) Seeing is deceiving: The psychology of visual illusions. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ Cornoldi C, Bertuccelli B, Rocchi P, Sbrana B (1993) Processing capacity limitations in pictorial and spatial representations in the totally congenitally blind. Cortex 29: 675-689 Cuijpers RH, Kappers AM, Koenderink JJ (2000) Large systematic deviations in visual parallelism. Perception 29: 1467-1482 Cuijpers RH, Kappers AM, Koenderink JJ (2002) Visual perception of collinearity. Perception & Psychophysics 64: 1467-1482 Day RH, Avery GC (1970) Absence of the horizontal- vertical illusion in haptic space. J Exp Psychol 83: 172-173 Day RH, Wong TS (1971) Radial and tangential movement directions as determinants of the haptic illusion in an L figure. J Exp Psychol 87: 19-22 Deregowski J, Ellis HD (1972) Effect of stimulus orientation upon haptic perception of the horizontal- vertical illusion. J Exp Psychol 95: 14-19 Drewing K, Ernst MO (2003) Integration of force and position cues in haptic curvature perception. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society 44th Annual Meeting: 115 Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrates visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415: 429-433 Faineteau H, Gentaz E, Viviani P (2003) The kinaesthetic estimation of Euclidean distances in blindfolded adults: A study on the detour path effect. Exp Brain Res 153: 166-172 Faineteau H, Gentaz E, Viviani P (2005) Factors affecting the size of the detour effect in the kinaesthetic perception of Euclidian distance. Exp Brain Res 163: 503-514 Gallace A, Spence C (2005) Examining the crossmodal consequences of viewing the Muller-Lyer illusion. Exp Brain Res 162: 490-496 Gentaz E, Camos V, Hatwell Y, Jacquet AY (2004) The haptic and visual Müller-Lyer illusions: correlation studies. Curr Psychol Letts (revue électronique) Gentaz E, Gaunet F (2006) L’inférence haptique d’une localisation spatiale chez les adultes et les enfants: étude de l’effet du trajet et de l’effet du délai dans une tâche de complétion de triangle. L’Année Psychologique 2: 167-190 Gentaz E, Hatwell Y (1996) Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation. Perception & Psychophysics 58: 1278-1292 Gentaz E, Hatwell Y (1998) The haptic oblique effect in the perception of rod orientation by blind adults. Perception & Psychophysics 60: 157-167 Gentaz E, Hatwell Y (1999) Role of memorisation conditions in the haptic processing of orientations and the ‘oblique effect’. Br J Psychol 90: 377-388 Gentaz E, Hatwell Y (2004) Geometrical haptic illusion: Role of exploratory movements in the Muller-Lyer, Vertical-Horizontal and Delboeuf illusions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 11: 31-40 Gregory R (1998) Eye and brain. The psychology of seeing (5th edition). Oxford University Press, London Hatwell Y (1960) Etude de quelques ilusions géométriques tactiles chez les aveugles. L’Année Psychologique 60: 11-27 Hatwell Y (1966) Privation sensorielle et intelligence. PUF, Paris Hatwell Y (1978) Form perception and related issues in blind humans. In: R Held, HW Leibowitz, HL Teuber (eds): Handbook of sensory physiology, VII: Perception. Springer Verlag, New York and Berlin Hatwell Y (1985) Piagetian reasoning and the blind (translated from Privation sensorielle et intelligence, 1966, Paris: Presses Universitaires de France). American Foundation for the Blind, New York Hatwell Y (1986) Toucher l’espace. Presses Universitaires de Lille, Lille Hatwell Y, Orliaguet JP, Brouty G (1990) Effects of object properties, attentional constraints and manual exploratory procedures on haptic perceptual organization: A developmental study. In: H Bloch, B Bertenthal (eds): Sensory-motor organization and development in infancy and early childhood. Klumer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht 315-335 Hatwell Y, Streri A, Gentaz E (2000) Toucher pour connaître. Psychologie cognitive de la perception tactile manuelle. PUF, Paris Hatwell Y, Streri A, Gentaz E (2003) Touching for knowing. John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam/Philadelphia Hayward V (2008) A brief taxonomy of tactile illusions and demonstrations that can be done in a hardware store. Brain Res Bulletin 75: 742-752 Heller M (ed) (2000) Touch, representation and blindness. Oxford University Press, Oxford Heller M, Brackett D, Wilson K, Yoneyama K, Boyer A, Steffen H (2002) The haptic Müller-Lyer illusion in sighted and blind people. Perception 31: 1263-1274 Heller M, Brakett D, Wilson K, Yoneyame K, Boyer A (2002) Visual experience and the haptic horizontal- vertical illusion. Br J Visual Impairment 20: 105-109 Heller M, Calcaterra J, Burson L, Green S (1997) The tactual horizontal-vertical illusion depends on radial motion of the entire arm. Perception & Psychophysics 59: 1297-1311 Heller M, McCarthy M, Schultz J, Greene J, Shanley M, Clark A, Skoczylas S, Procjuk I (2005) The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Muller-Lyer illusion. Perception 34: 1475-1500 Heller MA (1983) Haptic dominance in form perception with blurred vision. Perception 12: 607-613 Heller MA, Joyner TD (1993) Mechanisms in the haptic horizontal-vertical illusion: Evidence from sighted and blind subjects. Perception & Psychophysics 53: 422-428 Hershberger W, Misceo G (1996) Touch dominates haptic estimates of discordant visual-haptic size. Perception & Psychophysics 58: 1124-1132 Hillebrand F (1902) Theorie der scheinbaren Grösse bei binocularem Sehen [Theory of apparent size in binocular vision]. Denkschriften der Wiener Akademie, math.-nat. 72: 255-307 Kappers A (2002) Haptic perception of parallelity in the midsagittal plane. Acta Psychologica 109: 25-40 Kappers AM (1999) Large systematic deviations in the haptic perception of parallelity. Perception 28: 1001-1012 Kappers AM, Koenderink JJ (1999) Haptic perception of spatial relations. Perception 28: 781-795 Künnapas T (1957) Interocular differences in the vertical-horizontal illusion. Acta Psychologica 13: 253-259 Künnapas T (1957) The vertical-horizontal illusion and the visual field. J Exp Psychol 53: 405-407 Künnapas T (1957) Vertical-horizontal and the surrounding field. Acta Psychologica 13: 35-42 Künnapas TM (1955) An analysis of the “verticalhorizontal illusion“. J Exp Psychol 49: 134-140 Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL, Barber PO (1985) Spatial and movement-based heuristics for encoding pattern information through touch. J Exp Psychology: General 114: 33-49 Lederman SJ, Thorne G, Jones B (1986) Perception of texture by vision and touch: Multidimensionality and intersensory integration. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception & Performance 12: 169-180 Lucca A, Dellantonio A, Riggio L (1986) Some observations on the Poggendorf and the Müller- Lyer tactual illusions. Perception & Psychophysics 39: 374-380 McFarland J, Soechting JF (2007) Factors influencing the radial-tangential illusion in haptic perception. Exp Brain Res 178: 99-114 Millar S (1994) Understanding and representing space. Theory and evidence from studies with blind and sighted children. Clarendon Press, Oxford Millar S (2000) Vertical and bisection bias in active touch. Perception 29: 481-500 Millar S (2001) Illusions in reading maps by touch: Reducing distance errors. Br J Psychol 92: 643-657 Millar S, Al-Attar Z (2002) The Müller-Lyer illusion in touch and vision: implications for multisensory processes. Perception & Psychophysics 64 (3): 353-365 Misceo GF, Hershberger WA, Mancini RL (1999) Haptic estimates of discordant visual-haptic size vary developmentally. Perception & Psychophysics 61 (4): 608-614 Over R (1966) A comparison of haptic and visual judgments of some illusions. Am J Psychol 79: 590-595 Over R (1966) Context and movement as factors influencing haptic illusions. Australian J Psychol 3: 262-265 Over R (1967) Haptic illusions and inappropriate constancy scaling. Nature 214: 629 Patterson J, Deffenbacher K (1972) Haptic perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion by the blind. Perceptual and Motor Skills 35: 819-824 Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J (2000) Visual capture of touch: out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychological Science 11: 353-359 Piaget J (1957) Les mécanismes perceptifs. PUF, Paris Power RP, Graham A (1976) Dominance of touch by vision: Generalization of the hypothesis to a tactually experienced population. Perception 5: 161-166 Prinzmetal W, Gettleman L (1993) Vertical-horizontal illusion: One eye is better than two. Perception & Psychophysics 53: 81-88 Revesz G (1934) System der optischen und haptishen Raumtäuschungen. Zeitschrift für Physiologie 131: 296-375 Revesz G (1950) Psychology and art of the blind. Longmans Green, London Robles-de-la-Torre G, Hayward V (2001) Force can overcome object geometry in the perception of shape through active touch. Nature 412: 445-448 Rock I, Victor J (1964) Vision and touch: An experimentally induced conflict between the two senses. Science 143: 594-596 Rudel RG, Teuber HL (1963) Decrement of visual and haptic Müller-Lyer illusion on repeated trials: A study of crossmodal transfer. Quarterly J Exp Psychol 15: 125-131 Rudel RG, Teuber HL (1963) Discrimination of direction of line in children. J Comparative & Physiological Psychol 56: 892-898 Sanders D, Kappers AM (2007) Haptically straight lines. Perception 36: 1688-1697 Spence C, Walton M (2005) On the inability to ignore touch when responding to vision in the crossmodal congruency task. Acta Psychologica 118: 47-70 Suzuki K, Arashida R (1992) Geometrical haptic illusions revisited: haptic illusions compared with visual illusions. Perception & Psychophysics 52 (3): 329-335 Thinus-Blanc C, Gaunet F (1997) Representation of space in the blind: Vision as a spatial sense? Psychological Bulletin 121: 20-42 Tsai LS (1967) Mueller-Lyer illusion by the blind. Perceptual and Motor Skills 25: 641-644 Von Collani G (1979) An analysis of illusion components with L and T (inversed) figures in active touch. Quarterly J Exp Psychol 31: 241-248 Walkers JT (1971) Visual capture in illusions. Perception and Psychophysics 10: 71-73 Wenderoth P, Alais D (1990) Lack of evidence for a tactual Poggendorff illusion. Perception & Psychophysics 48: 243-242 Wong TS (1975) The respective role of limb and eye movements in the haptic and visual Müller- Lyer illusion. Quarterly J Exp Psychol 27: 659-666 Wong TS (1977) Dynamic properties of radial and tangential movements as determinants of the haptic horizontal-vertical illusion with an L figure. J Exp Psychol: Human Perception & Performance 3: 151-164 Wong TS (1979) Developmental study of a haptic illusion in relation to Piaget’s centration theory. J Exp Child Psychol 27: 489-500 Worchel P (1951) Space perception and orientation in the blind. Psychological Monograph 65 (15) Wydoodt P, Gentaz E, Streri A (2006) Role of force cues in the haptic estimations of a virtual length. Exp Brain Res 171: 489-489
<link>Ref: « Haptic object identification <link>Ref: » Haptic perception in interaction with other senses <link>references: chapters, <link>all <link>contents «Human Haptic Perception», Grunwald (Ed.)